Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (2024)

Article Navigation

Article Contents

  • Abstract

  • Introduction

  • Materials and methods

  • Results

  • Discussion

  • Funding

  • Conflict of interest

  • Research involving human participants

  • Informed consent

  • Data and code availability

  • References

Journal Article

,

Shiyu Wei

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Weipeng Jin

Department of Neurosurgery, Tianjin Huanhu Hospital

, Tianjin 300060,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Wenwei Zhu

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Shuning Chen

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Jie Feng

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Pinchun Wang

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Hohjin Im

Department of Psychological Science, University of California

, Irvine 92697-7085 CA,

USA

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Kun Deng

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Bin Zhang

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Manman Zhang

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students’ Mental Development and Learning, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

... Show more

,

Shaofeng Yang

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students’ Mental Development and Learning, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Maomiao Peng

Department of Psychology, University of Arizona

, Tucson 85721 AZ,

USA

Search for other works by this author on:

Qiang Wang

Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students’ Mental Development and Learning, Tianjin Normal University

, Tianjin 300387,

China

Correspondence should be addressed to Qiang Wang, Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387, China. E-mail: [email protected].

Search for other works by this author on:

contributed equally.

Author Notes

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2023, nsac046, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsac046

Published:

20 July 2022

Article history

Received:

20 April 2022

Revision received:

20 June 2022

Editorial decision:

18 July 2022

Accepted:

20 July 2022

Published:

20 July 2022

Corrected and typeset:

02 August 2022

  • Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (3)PDF
  • Split View
  • Views
    • Article contents
    • Figures & tables
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Supplementary Data
  • Cite

    Cite

    Shiyu Wei, Weipeng Jin, Wenwei Zhu, Shuning Chen, Jie Feng, Pinchun Wang, Hohjin Im, Kun Deng, Bin Zhang, Manman Zhang, Shaofeng Yang, Maomiao Peng, Qiang Wang, Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Volume 18, Issue 1, 2023, nsac046, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsac046

    Close

Search

Close

Search

Search Menu

Abstract

Greed personality trait (GPT), characterized by the desire to acquire more and the dissatisfaction of never having enough, has been hypothesized to link with negative emotion/affect characteristics and aggressive behaviors. To describe its emotion-related features, we utilized a series of scales to measure corresponding emotion/affect and aggression (n = 411) and collected their neuroimaging data (n = 330) to explore underlying morphological substrates. Correlational analyses revealed that greedy individuals show more negative symptoms (e.g. depression, loss of interest, negative affect), lower psychological well-being and more aggression. Mediation analyses further demonstrated that negative symptoms and psychological well-being mediated greedy individuals’ aggression. Moreover, exploratory factor analysis extracted factor scores across three factors (negative psychopathology, happiness, and motivation) from the measures scales. Negative psychopathology and happiness remained robust mediators. Importantly, these findings were replicated in an independent sample (n = 68). Voxel-based morphometry analysis also revealed that gray matter volumes (GMVs) in the prefrontal-parietal-occipital system were associated with negative psychopathology and happiness, and GMVs in the frontal pole and middle frontal cortex mediated the relationships between GPT and aggressions. These findings provide novel insights into the negative characteristics of dispositional greed, and suggest their mediating roles on greedy individuals’ aggression and underlying neuroanatomical substrates.

greed personality trait, happiness, negative psychopathology, aggression, voxel-based morphometry

Introduction

Greed personality trait (GPT) is often characterized by the experience of desiring more and the dissatisfaction of not having enough. Greed is not only associated with immorality and unethical behavior in philosophy and religion (Seuntjens etal., 2015a, 2019), but has also been considered to be a cause of many financial problems and scandals (Seuntjens etal., 2016, 2019). In particular, greed has been broadly believed to correlate positively with maximization tendencies (Seuntjens etal., 2015a), materialism (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015), egoism (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015) and selfishness (Lambie and Haugen, 2019). However, greed is not inherently negative. Economists have highlighted greed’s destructive role in financial crises (e.g. subprime mortgage crisis in the United States and debt crisis in Europe) (Mussel etal., 2015) but have also acknowledged its influence in motivating innovation and spurring economic growth (Williams, 2000; Fehr and Gintis, 2007), implying the duality of greed. Nevertheless, much of greed research has been at the behavioral level and its associated cognitive and neural mechanisms remain understudied. Understanding these mechanisms may be informative to how economical habits and moral behaviors are shaped.

Greed has been proposed to have stable negative consequences, especially for adverse emotion/affect experiences. The well-known ‘Tragedy of the Commons’ is one classic example of the negative consequence of greed on public resource allocation (Hardin, 1968). Considerable empirical evidence has also demonstrated that greedy individuals subjectively experience a series of negative emotions, including unhappiness (Seuntjens etal., 2015a), envy (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015), negative affect after losing money (Mussel and Hewig, 2016), life dissatisfaction (Pavot and Diener, 2009; Seuntjens etal., 2015b) and anger (Vrabel etal., 2019). Indirect evidence suggest that greed is positively associated with several personality traits, such as antagonism, disinhibition, detachment, negative affectivity and psychoticism (Seuntjens etal., 2019; Vrabel etal., 2019). Although the extant literature in the field has focused on the negative outcomes and behaviors of greed, only a few studies have directly explored the negative psychopathological core features of greed, particularly pertaining to symptoms of depression, anxiety and their mixed symptoms, which possibly limits the understanding of the nature of greed and its impacts on mental health. Thus, the present study was aimed to comprehensively delineate dispositional GPT via utilizing field-standard measures of psychopathology to improve our insights of greed.

Our first inquiry of greed’s negative association with subjective well-being begins with the investigation of the stable negative consequences of greed behaviors and its definition. Greedy individuals have been proposed to be dissatisfied with their current state of affairs, which in turn, deteriorates their self-esteem and life satisfaction (Seuntjens etal., 2015b). As well-being is multifaceted, consisting of social, psychological and subjective factors, it reflects distinct aspects of one’s happiness.

Greedy individuals often resort to aggressive measures to achieve their personal goals and desires (Winarick, 2010). Although the direct empirical evidence to support such view remains scarce, considerable indirect evidence hint at the potential relations between these two constructs. Negative emotional state typically precedes peripheral antagonistic states, such as anger, hostility and nervousness (Donahue etal., 2014; Kovácsová etal., 2016), that culminate into physical aggression (Garofalo and Velotti, 2017). Further, emotion dysregulation is believed to play a critical mediating role in the associations between negative emotion/affect and aggression (Velotti etal., 2019; Puhalla and McCloskey, 2020). Thus, we sought to directly examine whether greed is positively associated with aggressive tendencies.

Although studies on greed using survey methods are increasingly growing, supplemental investigations on the neural substrates underlying the greed are relatively rare. Evidence from electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have predominantly been concerned with how greedy individuals’ decision making and their underlying neural substrates. EEG studies on greed reported reduced feedback-related negativity difference between unfavorable and favorable outcomes (Mussel etal., 2015), and decreased P300 effect to positive feedback in greedy individuals (Mussel and Hewig, 2019), suggesting altered learning capacity from prior experiences and feedback. One functional MRI study also observed a neural mediating mechanism underlying the associations between GPT and behavioral loss aversion via activations in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) and medial orbitofrontal cortex (Li etal., 2019). Further, two recent fMRI studies directly examined the neuroanatomical and functional substrates of GPT combining univariate and multivariate pattern analysis approaches (Wang etal., 2021a, 2021c). At the morphological level, grey matter volumes (GMVs) on the lateral frontal pole cortex (FPC), VMPFC and lateral occipital cortex (LOC) were found to significantly predict individual variability on greed (Wang etal., 2021a). At the functional level, reward-related brain activations on the lateral OFC and prospection network system, including the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), dorsomedial PFC (DMPFC), superior parietal lobule (SPL) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) were associated with GPT scores (Wang etal., 2021a). Additionally, reward-related static and dynamic functional networks have been demonstrated to be important in supporting greed(Wang etal., 2021a). Taken together, the morphological and functional characteristics of the prefrontal cortex are associated with greed. Considering the possible link between GPT and aggression, we thus further explored the neural substrates underlying the associations between greed and aggression, and hypothesized that the prefrontal cortex subsurving into reward and prospective thinking may be a potential candidate region for understanding the effects of greed on social behaviors.

In the current study, we collected data utilizing a large number of questionnaires related to negative emotions, happiness, and social behavior, in addition to individual structural imaging data in a relatively large sample (n = 411), aiming to comprehensively depict the characteristics of dispostional greed and its associations to negative psychopathology and maladaptive social behaviors (i.e. aggression). Furthermore, we explored whether these negative psychopathological characteristics mediated the associations between greed and aggression. We then employed exploratory factor analyses (EFAs) to extract the common latent factors relevant to greed. Finally, we investigated the neuroanatomical substrates underlying these main factors and mediation effects on the GMV from a explorative perspective.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 497 college students participated in this study. Eighteen participants were removed from final analysis due to incomplete (n = 12) and low quality data (n = 6) for a final sample of 479 (64.5% females, age ranged from 17 to 28 years old). Participants were further divided into two datasets. The first included 411 participants (65.9% females, age M ± SD = 19.93 ± 1.47), whose results were reported in the main text. The second dataset included 68 participants (55.9% females, age M ± SD = 20.72 ± 1.74) which provided replication validation analysis. In the first dataset, 330 participants simultaneously had T1-weighted imaging data (sub-dataset 1). No participant self-reported any history of neurological or psychiatric issues. Written informed consent was obtained from all adult participants (age 18–28) before formal investigation. Five adolescent participants (age 17) were required to sign the consent form after receiving the verbal consent from their parents. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Tianjin Normal University (No. XL2020-27), China.

Measures and questionnaires

Greed personality trait

GPT was measured by the 7-item Dispositional Greed Scale (Seuntjens etal., 2015b; Mussel etal., 2018) where participants rated their degree of agreement with each statement describing greedy tendencies, α = 0.749.

Depression and anxiety

Depression was measured using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) using a 4-point continuum of statements representing the degree of severity of depression symptomology, α = 0.902. Capturing the tripartite model of both anxiety and depression was the Mood and Anxiety Symptoms Questionnaire (MASQ) (Clark and Watson, 1991), α = 0.922. The MASQ captures three aspects of general distress: (i) mixed symptoms (15 items), (ii) depressive symptoms (12 items) and (iii) anxious symptoms (11 items). Further, the MASQ also measures two additional sets of symptomologies: (iv) anxious arousal (17 items) and (v) anhedonic depression (22 items). Lastly, the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck etal., 1961) was used to measure severity of an individual’s experience of anxiety, α = 0.902.

Affect

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) was used to measure experience of positive and negative affect (Watson etal., 1988). Participants rated the degree to which they experienced different feelings and emotions across (i) Positive (10 items, e.g. ‘excited’) and (ii) Negative affect experiences (e.g. ‘distressed’) within the past week.

Aggression

Aggression was measured using the Buss-Warren Aggression Questionnaire (BWAQ) (Buss and Warren, 2000) and the Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ) (Raine etal., 2006). The BWAQ is a 34-item instrument that measures five dimensions of trait aggression: (i) physical aggression, (ii) verbal aggression, (iii) anger, (iv) hostility and (v) indirect aggression, α = 0.890. The RPQ is a 23-item instrument that measures trait tendencies to engage in (i) proactive aggression (α = 0.839, i.e. instigating aggression and antagonizing others) and (ii) reactive aggression (α = 0.825, i.e. impulsive responses to threat and provocation).

Well-being

Psychological well-being was measured with the 84-item Psychological Well-Being Scale (PWBS) (Ryff and Keyes, 1995) consisting of six distinct dimensions: (i) autonomy, (ii) environmental mastery, (iii) personal growth, (iv) positive relations, (v) purpose in life and (vi) self-acceptance, α = 0.801. Social well-being was measured using the 15-item Social Well-Being Scale (SWBS) (Keyes, 1998) consisting of five dimensions: (i) social integration, (ii) social contribution, (iii) social coherence, (iv) social actualization and (v) social acceptance, α = 0.864. Lastly, we captured subjective happiness using the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) (Lyubomirsky and Lepper, 1999). The SHS assesses an individual’s broad, global subjective happiness using both self-report and self-comparison items.

Work preference

Work preference was measured using the 30-item Work Preference Inventory (WPI) (Amabile etal., 1994) across two broad motivational orientations: (i) Intrinsic Motivation and (ii) Extrinsic Motivation. Intrinsic motivation comprised of two additional subdimensions capturing personal challenge and enjoyment while the Extrinsic motivation comprised of preferences for outward recognition and compensation, α = 0.810.

Brain imaging data acquisition

Whole-brain imaging data were collected using a Siemens 3 T Prisma scanner with a 64-channel head coil at the Center for MRI Research of Tianjin Normal University. Participants laid supine on the scanner bed with foam pads reduce and minimize head motion. High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were extracted using MP-RAGE sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2530 ms; echo time (TE) = 2.98 ms; multi-band factor = 2; flip angle = 7 degree; field-of-view (FOV) = 224 × 256 mm2; slices = 192; voxel size = 0.5 × 0.5 × 1.0 mm3.

Structural MRI preprocessing

Structural MRI data were preprocessed using the Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain Software Library voxel-based morphometry (FSL-VBM), a VBM style analysis toolbox implemented in FSL (version 6.0.0; part of the FSL package; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Structural images of brains were extracted, tissue-type segmented, and then aligned to the grey matter template in the MNI152 standard space. The spatially normalized images were averaged to create a study-custom template and the native grey matter images were registered again using both linear and non-linear algorithms. The registered partial volume images were modulated by dividing them with the Jacobian of the warp field to correct for local expansion or contraction. The modulated segmented images, which represented GMV, were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with 3 mm standard deviation.

Data analysis

First, bivariate correlational analyses were conducted to examine basic associations between the features related to GPT in both datasets. Linear regression analyses were employed to validate these significant associations controlling for relevant covariates (e.g. parental education, age and sex) partially due to their correlations with GPT in the current study and previous literatures (Liu etal., 2019; Jiang etal., 2020). Significant findings reported in the main text were robust after controlling for covariates and thus are not presented again.

Second, mediation models were run to examine whether negative psychopathology and happiness mediated the effects of GPT on aggression behaviors. Linear regression analysis was used to test the relation between (1) GPT and aggression behaviors (Y = a1 + b1X + ε1); (2) GPT and negative psychopathology/happiness (M = a2 + b2X + ε2); (3) GPT and aggression behaviors with a mediator (Y = a3 + b3X + bM + ε3). In these equations, Y represents the criterion variable, X the predictor variable, and M the mediator. The indirect effect was estimated as b2 × b and the bootstrap estimations (1000 resamples) were performed by using SPSS PROCESS v2.16.3 (Version 25.0) (Hayes, 2017) to obtain accurate statistical significance. Due to limited space, we provide the mediation-effect-related figures from the larger dataset but not the smaller dataset.

Third, two EFAs were conducted on the subscales related to GPT using SPSS (version 25.0): one on all the 411 participants and another on the 330 participants with high-quality imaging data. Varimax with Kaiser Normalization was employed to rotate the loading matrix, and regression analysis was used to calculate factor scores from each subscale.

Finally, we examined associations between the factors (i.e. negative psychopathology and happiness) and GMV at the whole-brain level using a mixed-effect FLAME 1 model implemented in FSL. Parental education, age at MRI scans, sex and total GMV were included as covariates. In the regression analyses, covariates were entered into the first block of variables. In the second block, mean-centered factor scores were entered. Statistical results were determined at a cluster level (z > 2.3, P < 0.01) and at family-wise error rate of 0.05 for the correction for multiple comparisons using Gaussian Random Field Theory (Wang etal., 2019b, 2020, 2021b).

Results

Demographics

Tables1 and 2 provide demographic information, each scale’s scores in both datasets, and their group comparisons. Minimal significant group differences were found between the two datasets, pertaining only to age (t(477) = −3.979, P < 0.001), anxious symptoms (t(477) = 4.556, P < 0.001), anxious arousal, (t(477) = 5.731, P < 0.001) and high positive affect (t(477) = −2.329, P = 0.020), suggesting that these two groups were closely hom*ogeneous as a whole and valid for cross-validation. In the first dataset (n = 411), the GPT scores ranged from 7 to 35 (M ± SD = 22.97 ± 4.15) with gender differences (t(409)= 2.48, P = 0.013). GPT did not vary by age (r = −0.053, P = 0.286) or maternal education level (r = 0.064, P = 0.199), but was weakly associated with paternal education level (r = 0.132, P = 0.007). In the second validation dataset (n = 68), the M ± SD of GPT were 23.30 ± 4.370. Gender differences were not observed in GPT (t(66) = −0.494, P = 0.623) and GPT was not correlated with age (r = 0.157, P = 0.201), maternal education level (r = −0.132, P = 0.285), or paternal education level (r = −0.234, P = 0.055). Due to non-normal distributions of the raw GPT scores, we used a rank-based inverse Gaussian transformation to convert the GPT scores (Wang etal., 2019a). All findings remained robust to the GPT score transformation except for paternal education level that became significantly associated with GPT in the second dataset, albeit the associated change was minimal (i.e. P value from 0.055 to 0.039). All subsequent analyses were thus conducted using the transformed GPT scores.

Table1.

Sample demographics

MeasuresDataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
t/Χ2P
Gender (Male/Female)140/27130/382.5760.108
Age (M ± SD)19.93 ± 1.4720.72 ± 1.74−3.9798.0e−5
Paternal education (%)3.5630.614
Less than primary school12.713.0
Junior high school38.242.0
Vocational high School16.117.4
Senior high school11.213.0
Junior college education9.02.9
Undergraduate level12.910.1
Maternal education (%)9.6720.085
Less than primary school17.024.6
Junior high school36.042.0
Vocational high school14.64.3
Senior high school12.911.6
Junior college education10.04.3
Undergraduate level9.411.6
MeasuresDataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
t/Χ2P
Gender (Male/Female)140/27130/382.5760.108
Age (M ± SD)19.93 ± 1.4720.72 ± 1.74−3.9798.0e−5
Paternal education (%)3.5630.614
Less than primary school12.713.0
Junior high school38.242.0
Vocational high School16.117.4
Senior high school11.213.0
Junior college education9.02.9
Undergraduate level12.910.1
Maternal education (%)9.6720.085
Less than primary school17.024.6
Junior high school36.042.0
Vocational high school14.64.3
Senior high school12.911.6
Junior college education10.04.3
Undergraduate level9.411.6

Abbreviations: M, mean score; SD, standard deviation.

Table1.

Sample demographics

MeasuresDataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
t/Χ2P
Gender (Male/Female)140/27130/382.5760.108
Age (M ± SD)19.93 ± 1.4720.72 ± 1.74−3.9798.0e−5
Paternal education (%)3.5630.614
Less than primary school12.713.0
Junior high school38.242.0
Vocational high School16.117.4
Senior high school11.213.0
Junior college education9.02.9
Undergraduate level12.910.1
Maternal education (%)9.6720.085
Less than primary school17.024.6
Junior high school36.042.0
Vocational high school14.64.3
Senior high school12.911.6
Junior college education10.04.3
Undergraduate level9.411.6
MeasuresDataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
t/Χ2P
Gender (Male/Female)140/27130/382.5760.108
Age (M ± SD)19.93 ± 1.4720.72 ± 1.74−3.9798.0e−5
Paternal education (%)3.5630.614
Less than primary school12.713.0
Junior high school38.242.0
Vocational high School16.117.4
Senior high school11.213.0
Junior college education9.02.9
Undergraduate level12.910.1
Maternal education (%)9.6720.085
Less than primary school17.024.6
Junior high school36.042.0
Vocational high school14.64.3
Senior high school12.911.6
Junior college education10.04.3
Undergraduate level9.411.6

Abbreviations: M, mean score; SD, standard deviation.

Table2.

Basic questionnaires’ scores

MeasuresDataset 1(n = 411)Dataset 2 (n = 68)tP
DGS22.97 ± 4.1523.29 ± 4.33−0.5950.552
SWLS18.67 ± 5.7918.06 ± 6.690.7070.482
MASQ
Mixed symptoms35.47 ± 9.7235.37 ± 10.920.0770.939
Depressive25.03 ± 11.1622.24 ± 9.141.9610.050
Anxious symptoms20.67 ± 8.8516.50 ± 6.634.5561.3e−5
Loss of interest18.35 ± 6.1517.03 ± 6.671.6230.105
Anxious arousal28.97 ± 9.2723.68 ± 6.625.7318.09e−8
High positive affect57.36 ± 21.4963.82 ± 19.42−2.3290.020
SHS19.11 ± 4.9018.54 ± 5.580.8590.391
PWBS
Positive relations58.86 ± 9.3158.34 ± 10.050.4200.675
Autonomy51.74 ± 8.1551.09 ± 9.080.6010.548
Environmental mastery54.55 ± 8.2653.91 ± 9.800.5120.610
Personal growth60.91 ± 7.8659.60 ± 7.651.2740.203
Purpose in life57.58 ± 9.8556.59 ± 10.260.7620.446
Self-acceptance52.14 ± 9.3356.59 ± 10.48−0.3630.716
BDI9.05 ± 9.218.90 ± 7.590.1470.883
PANAS
Positive affect27.67 ± 7.7325.81 ± 8.091.8260.068
Negative affect16.94 ± 6.2115.97 ± 5.631.2030.230
WPI
Enjoy13.89 ± 6.7113.71 ± 5.920.2110.833
Challenge−0.23 ± 4.680.18 ± 4.54−0.6680.504
Outward7.64 ± 5.327.54 ± 4.720.1360.892
Compensation4.39 ± 3.994.43 ± 3.34−0.0770.938
Intrinsic13.66 ± 9.5213.88 ± 8.31−0.1830.855
Extrinsic12.02 ± 7.4911.97 ± 6.680.0560.956
BWAQ
Physical16.61 ± 5.8216.57 ± 5.110.0430.966
Verbal12.84 ± 3.2712.01 ± 3.051.9390.053
Anger16.18 ± 4.0116.07 ± 3.920.1940.846
Hostility20.51 ± 4.8920.38 ± 5.140.2030.839
Indirect14.22 ± 3.8813.79 ± 3.830.8380.403
Total score80.35 ± 17.0878.84 ± 17.120.6760.499
RPQ
Reactive aggression6.07 ± 4.136.96 ± 3.42−1.9220.057
Proactive aggression0.71 ± 1.820.51 ± 1.310.8300.407
SWBS72.78 ± 11.3972.93 ± 13.050.2500.803
MeasuresDataset 1(n = 411)Dataset 2 (n = 68)tP
DGS22.97 ± 4.1523.29 ± 4.33−0.5950.552
SWLS18.67 ± 5.7918.06 ± 6.690.7070.482
MASQ
Mixed symptoms35.47 ± 9.7235.37 ± 10.920.0770.939
Depressive25.03 ± 11.1622.24 ± 9.141.9610.050
Anxious symptoms20.67 ± 8.8516.50 ± 6.634.5561.3e−5
Loss of interest18.35 ± 6.1517.03 ± 6.671.6230.105
Anxious arousal28.97 ± 9.2723.68 ± 6.625.7318.09e−8
High positive affect57.36 ± 21.4963.82 ± 19.42−2.3290.020
SHS19.11 ± 4.9018.54 ± 5.580.8590.391
PWBS
Positive relations58.86 ± 9.3158.34 ± 10.050.4200.675
Autonomy51.74 ± 8.1551.09 ± 9.080.6010.548
Environmental mastery54.55 ± 8.2653.91 ± 9.800.5120.610
Personal growth60.91 ± 7.8659.60 ± 7.651.2740.203
Purpose in life57.58 ± 9.8556.59 ± 10.260.7620.446
Self-acceptance52.14 ± 9.3356.59 ± 10.48−0.3630.716
BDI9.05 ± 9.218.90 ± 7.590.1470.883
PANAS
Positive affect27.67 ± 7.7325.81 ± 8.091.8260.068
Negative affect16.94 ± 6.2115.97 ± 5.631.2030.230
WPI
Enjoy13.89 ± 6.7113.71 ± 5.920.2110.833
Challenge−0.23 ± 4.680.18 ± 4.54−0.6680.504
Outward7.64 ± 5.327.54 ± 4.720.1360.892
Compensation4.39 ± 3.994.43 ± 3.34−0.0770.938
Intrinsic13.66 ± 9.5213.88 ± 8.31−0.1830.855
Extrinsic12.02 ± 7.4911.97 ± 6.680.0560.956
BWAQ
Physical16.61 ± 5.8216.57 ± 5.110.0430.966
Verbal12.84 ± 3.2712.01 ± 3.051.9390.053
Anger16.18 ± 4.0116.07 ± 3.920.1940.846
Hostility20.51 ± 4.8920.38 ± 5.140.2030.839
Indirect14.22 ± 3.8813.79 ± 3.830.8380.403
Total score80.35 ± 17.0878.84 ± 17.120.6760.499
RPQ
Reactive aggression6.07 ± 4.136.96 ± 3.42−1.9220.057
Proactive aggression0.71 ± 1.820.51 ± 1.310.8300.407
SWBS72.78 ± 11.3972.93 ± 13.050.2500.803

Table2.

Basic questionnaires’ scores

MeasuresDataset 1(n = 411)Dataset 2 (n = 68)tP
DGS22.97 ± 4.1523.29 ± 4.33−0.5950.552
SWLS18.67 ± 5.7918.06 ± 6.690.7070.482
MASQ
Mixed symptoms35.47 ± 9.7235.37 ± 10.920.0770.939
Depressive25.03 ± 11.1622.24 ± 9.141.9610.050
Anxious symptoms20.67 ± 8.8516.50 ± 6.634.5561.3e−5
Loss of interest18.35 ± 6.1517.03 ± 6.671.6230.105
Anxious arousal28.97 ± 9.2723.68 ± 6.625.7318.09e−8
High positive affect57.36 ± 21.4963.82 ± 19.42−2.3290.020
SHS19.11 ± 4.9018.54 ± 5.580.8590.391
PWBS
Positive relations58.86 ± 9.3158.34 ± 10.050.4200.675
Autonomy51.74 ± 8.1551.09 ± 9.080.6010.548
Environmental mastery54.55 ± 8.2653.91 ± 9.800.5120.610
Personal growth60.91 ± 7.8659.60 ± 7.651.2740.203
Purpose in life57.58 ± 9.8556.59 ± 10.260.7620.446
Self-acceptance52.14 ± 9.3356.59 ± 10.48−0.3630.716
BDI9.05 ± 9.218.90 ± 7.590.1470.883
PANAS
Positive affect27.67 ± 7.7325.81 ± 8.091.8260.068
Negative affect16.94 ± 6.2115.97 ± 5.631.2030.230
WPI
Enjoy13.89 ± 6.7113.71 ± 5.920.2110.833
Challenge−0.23 ± 4.680.18 ± 4.54−0.6680.504
Outward7.64 ± 5.327.54 ± 4.720.1360.892
Compensation4.39 ± 3.994.43 ± 3.34−0.0770.938
Intrinsic13.66 ± 9.5213.88 ± 8.31−0.1830.855
Extrinsic12.02 ± 7.4911.97 ± 6.680.0560.956
BWAQ
Physical16.61 ± 5.8216.57 ± 5.110.0430.966
Verbal12.84 ± 3.2712.01 ± 3.051.9390.053
Anger16.18 ± 4.0116.07 ± 3.920.1940.846
Hostility20.51 ± 4.8920.38 ± 5.140.2030.839
Indirect14.22 ± 3.8813.79 ± 3.830.8380.403
Total score80.35 ± 17.0878.84 ± 17.120.6760.499
RPQ
Reactive aggression6.07 ± 4.136.96 ± 3.42−1.9220.057
Proactive aggression0.71 ± 1.820.51 ± 1.310.8300.407
SWBS72.78 ± 11.3972.93 ± 13.050.2500.803
MeasuresDataset 1(n = 411)Dataset 2 (n = 68)tP
DGS22.97 ± 4.1523.29 ± 4.33−0.5950.552
SWLS18.67 ± 5.7918.06 ± 6.690.7070.482
MASQ
Mixed symptoms35.47 ± 9.7235.37 ± 10.920.0770.939
Depressive25.03 ± 11.1622.24 ± 9.141.9610.050
Anxious symptoms20.67 ± 8.8516.50 ± 6.634.5561.3e−5
Loss of interest18.35 ± 6.1517.03 ± 6.671.6230.105
Anxious arousal28.97 ± 9.2723.68 ± 6.625.7318.09e−8
High positive affect57.36 ± 21.4963.82 ± 19.42−2.3290.020
SHS19.11 ± 4.9018.54 ± 5.580.8590.391
PWBS
Positive relations58.86 ± 9.3158.34 ± 10.050.4200.675
Autonomy51.74 ± 8.1551.09 ± 9.080.6010.548
Environmental mastery54.55 ± 8.2653.91 ± 9.800.5120.610
Personal growth60.91 ± 7.8659.60 ± 7.651.2740.203
Purpose in life57.58 ± 9.8556.59 ± 10.260.7620.446
Self-acceptance52.14 ± 9.3356.59 ± 10.48−0.3630.716
BDI9.05 ± 9.218.90 ± 7.590.1470.883
PANAS
Positive affect27.67 ± 7.7325.81 ± 8.091.8260.068
Negative affect16.94 ± 6.2115.97 ± 5.631.2030.230
WPI
Enjoy13.89 ± 6.7113.71 ± 5.920.2110.833
Challenge−0.23 ± 4.680.18 ± 4.54−0.6680.504
Outward7.64 ± 5.327.54 ± 4.720.1360.892
Compensation4.39 ± 3.994.43 ± 3.34−0.0770.938
Intrinsic13.66 ± 9.5213.88 ± 8.31−0.1830.855
Extrinsic12.02 ± 7.4911.97 ± 6.680.0560.956
BWAQ
Physical16.61 ± 5.8216.57 ± 5.110.0430.966
Verbal12.84 ± 3.2712.01 ± 3.051.9390.053
Anger16.18 ± 4.0116.07 ± 3.920.1940.846
Hostility20.51 ± 4.8920.38 ± 5.140.2030.839
Indirect14.22 ± 3.8813.79 ± 3.830.8380.403
Total score80.35 ± 17.0878.84 ± 17.120.6760.499
RPQ
Reactive aggression6.07 ± 4.136.96 ± 3.42−1.9220.057
Proactive aggression0.71 ± 1.820.51 ± 1.310.8300.407
SWBS72.78 ± 11.3972.93 ± 13.050.2500.803

Test of common method bias

Prior to formal analysis, we first examined whether the results were influenced by common method bias using the Harman’s single-factor test (Podsakoff etal., 2003). This test indicated that the variance explained by the first factor was below the critical 40% threshold (unrotated factor solution: 36.30%; rotated factor solution: 22.27%) in the first dataset and the critical 50% threshold (unrotated factor solution: 43.46%; rotated factor solution: 26.77%) in the second dataset. Both these findings suggest non-significant common method bias effect within these two datasets (Hair, 2009).

GPT’s association with greater negative emotions

Table3 provides correlation results between GPT and the variables of interest. Pertaining to MASQ, GPT was positively correlated with depressive symptoms (r = 0.156, P = 0.001), anxious symptoms (r = 0.125, P = 0.011), mixed symptoms (r = 0.178, P < 0.001) and loss of interest (r = 0.143, P = 0.004) in the first dataset. These associations were replicated in the second dataset (depressive symptoms: r = 0.341, P = 0.004; mixed symptoms: r = 0.482, P < 0.001; loss of interest: r = 0.323, P = 0.007) except for anxious symptoms (r = 0.229, P = 0.061). Anxious arousal showed no associations with GPT in either datasets (all Pvalues> 0.054). High positive affect was negatively correlated with GPT in the second (r = −0.275, P = 0.023) but not in the first dataset (r = −0.020, P = 0.686).

Table3.

Pearson’s correlation coefficients with GPT scores

Dataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
GPTrPrP
EmotionMASQMixed symptoms0.1782.86e−40.4823.1e−5
Depressive symptoms0.1560.0010.3410.004
Anxious symptoms0.1250.0110.2290.061
Loss of interest0.1430.0040.3230.007
Anxious arousal0.0210.6730.2350.054
High positive affect−0.0200.686−0.2750.023
BDIBDI0.1410.0040.3200.008
BAIBAI0.1080.0280.1470.231
PANASPositive affect−0.0190.708−0.2270.063
Negative affect0.1500.0020.3640.002
HappinessSHSSHS−0.1040.035−0.1950.110
SWBSSWBS−0.0590.233−0.0870.478
PWBSPositive relations−0.1630.001−0.3980.001
Autonomy−0.1100.026−0.4010.001
Environmental mastery−0.1260.010−0.3380.005
Personal growth−0.1851.62 e−4−0.3490.004
Purpose in life−0.1871.35 e−4−0.3800.001
Self-acceptance−0.2052.9 e−5−0.3670.002
AggressionBWAQPhysical aggression0.2111.6 e−50.2730.025
Verbal aggression0.1985.5 e−50.3160.009
Anger0.1822.13 e−40.4213.54 e−4
Hostility0.3297.29e−120.4951.7 e−5
Indirect aggression0.1975.9 e−50.4193.80 e−4
Total aggression0.2911.74e−90.4764.0 e−5
RPQReactive aggression0.2023.7 e−50.3160.009
Proactive aggression0.1280.010.2220.068
MotivationWPIEnjoy−0.0720.146−0.1570.202
Challenge−0.0970.050−0.3630.002
Outward0.1540.0020.2880.017
Compensation0.1310.0080.0040.976
Intrinsic motivation−0.0980.047−0.2920.016
Extrinsic motivation0.1792.67e−40.2060.092
Dataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
GPTrPrP
EmotionMASQMixed symptoms0.1782.86e−40.4823.1e−5
Depressive symptoms0.1560.0010.3410.004
Anxious symptoms0.1250.0110.2290.061
Loss of interest0.1430.0040.3230.007
Anxious arousal0.0210.6730.2350.054
High positive affect−0.0200.686−0.2750.023
BDIBDI0.1410.0040.3200.008
BAIBAI0.1080.0280.1470.231
PANASPositive affect−0.0190.708−0.2270.063
Negative affect0.1500.0020.3640.002
HappinessSHSSHS−0.1040.035−0.1950.110
SWBSSWBS−0.0590.233−0.0870.478
PWBSPositive relations−0.1630.001−0.3980.001
Autonomy−0.1100.026−0.4010.001
Environmental mastery−0.1260.010−0.3380.005
Personal growth−0.1851.62 e−4−0.3490.004
Purpose in life−0.1871.35 e−4−0.3800.001
Self-acceptance−0.2052.9 e−5−0.3670.002
AggressionBWAQPhysical aggression0.2111.6 e−50.2730.025
Verbal aggression0.1985.5 e−50.3160.009
Anger0.1822.13 e−40.4213.54 e−4
Hostility0.3297.29e−120.4951.7 e−5
Indirect aggression0.1975.9 e−50.4193.80 e−4
Total aggression0.2911.74e−90.4764.0 e−5
RPQReactive aggression0.2023.7 e−50.3160.009
Proactive aggression0.1280.010.2220.068
MotivationWPIEnjoy−0.0720.146−0.1570.202
Challenge−0.0970.050−0.3630.002
Outward0.1540.0020.2880.017
Compensation0.1310.0080.0040.976
Intrinsic motivation−0.0980.047−0.2920.016
Extrinsic motivation0.1792.67e−40.2060.092

Notes: Bold represents significant correlations between GPT and sub-dimensions of scales in both two datasets.

Table3.

Pearson’s correlation coefficients with GPT scores

Dataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
GPTrPrP
EmotionMASQMixed symptoms0.1782.86e−40.4823.1e−5
Depressive symptoms0.1560.0010.3410.004
Anxious symptoms0.1250.0110.2290.061
Loss of interest0.1430.0040.3230.007
Anxious arousal0.0210.6730.2350.054
High positive affect−0.0200.686−0.2750.023
BDIBDI0.1410.0040.3200.008
BAIBAI0.1080.0280.1470.231
PANASPositive affect−0.0190.708−0.2270.063
Negative affect0.1500.0020.3640.002
HappinessSHSSHS−0.1040.035−0.1950.110
SWBSSWBS−0.0590.233−0.0870.478
PWBSPositive relations−0.1630.001−0.3980.001
Autonomy−0.1100.026−0.4010.001
Environmental mastery−0.1260.010−0.3380.005
Personal growth−0.1851.62 e−4−0.3490.004
Purpose in life−0.1871.35 e−4−0.3800.001
Self-acceptance−0.2052.9 e−5−0.3670.002
AggressionBWAQPhysical aggression0.2111.6 e−50.2730.025
Verbal aggression0.1985.5 e−50.3160.009
Anger0.1822.13 e−40.4213.54 e−4
Hostility0.3297.29e−120.4951.7 e−5
Indirect aggression0.1975.9 e−50.4193.80 e−4
Total aggression0.2911.74e−90.4764.0 e−5
RPQReactive aggression0.2023.7 e−50.3160.009
Proactive aggression0.1280.010.2220.068
MotivationWPIEnjoy−0.0720.146−0.1570.202
Challenge−0.0970.050−0.3630.002
Outward0.1540.0020.2880.017
Compensation0.1310.0080.0040.976
Intrinsic motivation−0.0980.047−0.2920.016
Extrinsic motivation0.1792.67e−40.2060.092
Dataset 1
(n = 411)
Dataset 2
(n = 68)
GPTrPrP
EmotionMASQMixed symptoms0.1782.86e−40.4823.1e−5
Depressive symptoms0.1560.0010.3410.004
Anxious symptoms0.1250.0110.2290.061
Loss of interest0.1430.0040.3230.007
Anxious arousal0.0210.6730.2350.054
High positive affect−0.0200.686−0.2750.023
BDIBDI0.1410.0040.3200.008
BAIBAI0.1080.0280.1470.231
PANASPositive affect−0.0190.708−0.2270.063
Negative affect0.1500.0020.3640.002
HappinessSHSSHS−0.1040.035−0.1950.110
SWBSSWBS−0.0590.233−0.0870.478
PWBSPositive relations−0.1630.001−0.3980.001
Autonomy−0.1100.026−0.4010.001
Environmental mastery−0.1260.010−0.3380.005
Personal growth−0.1851.62 e−4−0.3490.004
Purpose in life−0.1871.35 e−4−0.3800.001
Self-acceptance−0.2052.9 e−5−0.3670.002
AggressionBWAQPhysical aggression0.2111.6 e−50.2730.025
Verbal aggression0.1985.5 e−50.3160.009
Anger0.1822.13 e−40.4213.54 e−4
Hostility0.3297.29e−120.4951.7 e−5
Indirect aggression0.1975.9 e−50.4193.80 e−4
Total aggression0.2911.74e−90.4764.0 e−5
RPQReactive aggression0.2023.7 e−50.3160.009
Proactive aggression0.1280.010.2220.068
MotivationWPIEnjoy−0.0720.146−0.1570.202
Challenge−0.0970.050−0.3630.002
Outward0.1540.0020.2880.017
Compensation0.1310.0080.0040.976
Intrinsic motivation−0.0980.047−0.2920.016
Extrinsic motivation0.1792.67e−40.2060.092

Notes: Bold represents significant correlations between GPT and sub-dimensions of scales in both two datasets.

In investigating GPT’s associations with depression and anxiety, GPT was positively correlated with depression in both first (depression, r = 0.141, P = 0.004; negative affect, r = 0.150, P = 0.002) and second (depression, r = 0.320, P = 0.008; negative affect, r = 0.364, P = 0.002) dataset. GPT was positively correlated with anxiety in the first dataset (r = 0.108, P = 0.028) but not in the second dataset (r = 0.147, P = 0.231). No significant association was observed for positive affect (all P values > 0.063) in either datasets.

Regarding loss of interest, GPT was negatively correlated with the dimensions of challenge (all P values < 0.05) and intrinsic motivation (all P values < 0.05) while positively correlated with the outward motivation dimension (all P values < 0.05) in both datasets. Individuals with higher GPT scores exhibited more compensation (r = 0.131, P = 0.008) and extrinsic (r = 0.179, P < 0.001) motivation in the first dataset but not in the second dataset (all P values > 0.092).

GPT’s association with lower happiness

Negative associations of GPT with psychological well-being in all sub-scales of the measurements used (details in Table2) were generally observed across both datasets (Table3). GPT was negatively correlated with positive relations (all P values < 0.001), autonomy (all P values < 0.05), environment (all P values < 0.05), personal growth (all P values < 0.005), purpose in life (all P values < 0.001) and self-acceptance (all P values < 0.005) in both two datasets. However, a negative association between GPT and subjective happiness was only observed in the first dataset (r = -0.104, P = 0.035). No any other associations were observed (all P values > 0.1).

GPT’s association with greater aggressive behaviors

Pertaining to BWAQ, we observed significant associations of GPT with physical aggression (all P values < 0.05), verbal aggression (all P values < 0.01), anger (all P values < 0.001), hostility (all P values< 0.001), indirect aggression (all P values < 0.001) and aggregate scores (all P values < 0.001) in both two datasets (Table3). In relation to RPQ, GPT was positively correlated to reactive aggression (r = 0.202, P < 0.001) and proactive (r = 0.128, P = 0.009) aggression in the first dataset. We observed a similar pattern in reactive aggression (r = 0.316, P = 0.009) but only marginal significance in proactive aggression (r = 0.222, P = 0.068) in the second dataset.

Negative psychopathology symptoms modulate the association between GPT and aggression

Mixed symptom mediated the effects of GPT on physical aggression, anger, hostility and aggregate aggression score in BWAQ (Figure1A). In particular, mixed symptoms respectively partially and fully mediated the associations between GPT and physical aggression in the first (indirect effect = 0.020, 95% CI [0.003, 0.050]) and second dataset (indirect effect = 0.117, 95% CI [0.008, 0.298]). In addition, we observed similar mediation effects in anger (all indirect effects > 0.050, 95% CI [0.021, 0.346]) and hostility (all indirect effects > 0.050, 95% CI [0.027, 0.324]), as well as aggregate aggression scores (all indirect effects > 0.045, 95% CI [0.021, 0.333]).

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (4)

Fig.1.

Mediation models of negative symptoms (including depressive symptoms (A), mixed symptoms (B) and loss of interest (C)) on the relationships between GPT and aggression behaviors (including the reactive, physical, anger, hostility and total scores of BWAQ).

Depression symptom likewise showed similar mediation effects (Figure1B). Specifically, depression mediated the effects of GPT on reactive aggression (all indirect effects > 0.051, 95% CI [0.006, 0.206]), anger (all indirect effects > 0.041, 95% CI [0.017, 0.258]), hostility (all indirect effects > 0.067, 95% CI [0.031, 0.257]), as well as aggregate aggression scores of BWAQ (all indirect effects > 0.042, 95% CI [0.014, 0.251]). Moreover, we found the similar mediation effects of depression as measured by BDI on anger (all indirect effects > 0.040, 95% CI [0.015,0.242]), hostility (all indirect effects > 0.060, 95% CI [0.023, 0.288]) and aggregate aggression scores of BWAQ (all indirect effects > 0.037, 95% CI [0.014, 0.224]) (Figure2B).

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (5)

Fig.2.

Mediation models of negative affect and depression on the associations between GPT and aggression (including reactive aggression, physical, indirect, anger, hostility, total aggression of BWAQ).

Thirdly, pertaining to the loss of interest subdimension, we observed similar mediation effects as depression (Figure1C). Mediation analyses revealed that loss of interest mediated the underlying effects of GPT on reactive aggression in RPQ (all indirect effects > 0.041, 95% CI [0.001,0.170])), anger (all indirect effects > 0.037, 95% CI [0.013, 0.238]) and hostility (all indirect effects > 0.046, 95% CI [0.017, 0.249]).

In addition, we found that negative affect mediated more GPT effects on aggression (i.e. reactive aggression, physical aggression, anger, hostility and indirect aggression) than the aforementioned mediator variables (Figure2A). Paritcularly, negative affect mediated the effects of GPT on reactive aggression (all indirect effects > 0.054, 95% CI [0.021, 0.241]), physical aggression (all indirect effects > 0.033, 95% CI [0.012, 0.235]), anger (all indirect effects > 0.049, 95% CI [0.018, 0.334], hostility (all indirect effects > 0.060, 95%CI [0.025, 0.350]), indirect aggression (all indirect effects > 0.036, 95% CI [0.013, 0.292]) and aggregate scores of BWAQ (all indirect effects > 0.054, 95% CI [0.022,0.341]) across both datasets.

Psychological well-being modulates the associations between GPT and aggression

Pertaining to reactive aggression (RPQ) and physical aggression (BWAQ), we observed significant mediating roles of purpose in life, a sub-dimension of psychological well-being, on the effect of GPT in both datasets (For reactive aggression, all indirect effects > 0.020, 95% CI [0.002, 0.188]; For physical aggression, all indirect effects > 0.040, 95% CI [0.001, 0.193]) (Figure3D).

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (6)

Fig.3.

Mediation models of PWBS sub-dimensions (i.e. positive relations with others, personal growth and self-acceptance) on the associations between GPT and aggression (including anger (A), hostility (B), total aggression of BWAQ (C), reactive/physical aggression (D) and indirect aggression €).

Secondly, for anger (BWAQ), nearly all sub-dimensions of PWBS mediated the effect of GPT, except for autonomy (Figure3A). Specifically, the mediation effects were significant for positive relations (all indirect effects > 0.039, 95% CI [0.015, 0.308]), environmental mastery (all indirect effects > 0.039, 95% CI [0.010, 0.328]), personal growth (all indirect effects > 0.052, 95% CI [0.024, 0.296]), purpose in life (all indirect effects > 0.054, 95% CI [0.025, 0.331]), and self-acceptance (all indirect effects > 0.052, 95% CI [0.025, 0.313]).

Thirdly, for hostility (BWAQ), we likewise observed that all sub-dimensions of PWBS served as mediators in both datasets (Figure3B). Specifically, the mediation effects were significant for positive relations (all indirect effects > 0.056, 95% CI [0.023, 0.370]), autonomy (all indirect effects > 0.031, 95% CI [0.001,0.342]), environmental mastery (all indirect effects > 0.049, 95% CI [0.013, 0.334]), personal growth (all indirect effects > 0.045, 95% CI [0.021, 0.280]), purpose in life (all indirect effects > 0.064, 95% CI [0.029, 0.344]) and self-acceptance (all indirect effects > 0.092, 95% CI [0.052, 0.393]).

Fourthly, for indirect aggression (BWAQ), we found that only environmental mastery (all indirect effects > 0.022, 95% CI [0.005, 0.271]) and purpose in life (all indirect effects > 0.027, 95% CI [0.007, 0,305]) exhibited significantly partial mediation effects on the effect of GPT (Figure3E).

Finally, for the aggregate scores of aggression in the BWAQ, almost all sub-dimensions of PWBS exhibited the mediation effects on the associations between it and GPT (Figure3C). Specifically, mediation effects were found for positive relations (all indirect effects > 0.043, 95% CI [0.017, 0.272]), environmental mastery (all indirect effects > 0.036, 95% CI [0.009, 0.290]), personal growth (all indirect effects > 0.040, 95% CI [0.017, 0.256]), purpose in life (all indirect effects > 0.059, 95% CI [0.027, 0.303]) and self-acceptance (all indirect effects > 0.051, 95% CI [0.025, 0.301]) in both datasets.

Three major factors and their relations with GPT

The aforementioned analyses consistently demonstrated the close associations of GPT with negative psychopathology (i.e. mixed, depression symptoms, loss of interest, and negative affect), happiness (i.e. psychological well-being) and motivation. To further investigate patterns of effect, we further extracted the key factors between these measures using EFA for additional analyses.

Bartlett’s test of sphericity (χ2 = 3272.42, P < 0.001) and the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin test (value = 0.88) suggested the data were suitable for EFA. The 13 measures that exhibited significant correlations with GPT and had mediation effects in dataset 1 were entered in the analysis. Three factors were extracted based on the criteria of eigenvalues > 1 and variance explained > 60%: (i) negative psychopathology (fivesubscales), (ii) happiness (six subscales) and (iii) motivation (two subscales), which accounted for 70.74% of the total variance of the items. Table4 displays the varimax rotated factor loadings greater than 0.40.

Table4.

Exploratory factor analysis of the 13 subscales revealed three major factors

Sub-dimensionsNegative
psychopathology
HappinessMotivation
Mixed symptoms0.75
Depressive symptoms0.87
Loss of interest0.77
BDI0.78
PANAS_NA0.73
Positive relations0.82
Autonomy0.420.54
Environmental mastery−0.440.74
Personal growth0.74
Purpose in life0.81
Self-acceptance−0.530.57
Challenge0.92
Intrinsic motivation0.87
Sub-dimensionsNegative
psychopathology
HappinessMotivation
Mixed symptoms0.75
Depressive symptoms0.87
Loss of interest0.77
BDI0.78
PANAS_NA0.73
Positive relations0.82
Autonomy0.420.54
Environmental mastery−0.440.74
Personal growth0.74
Purpose in life0.81
Self-acceptance−0.530.57
Challenge0.92
Intrinsic motivation0.87

Note: Factor loadings below 0.40 not shown.

Table4.

Exploratory factor analysis of the 13 subscales revealed three major factors

Sub-dimensionsNegative
psychopathology
HappinessMotivation
Mixed symptoms0.75
Depressive symptoms0.87
Loss of interest0.77
BDI0.78
PANAS_NA0.73
Positive relations0.82
Autonomy0.420.54
Environmental mastery−0.440.74
Personal growth0.74
Purpose in life0.81
Self-acceptance−0.530.57
Challenge0.92
Intrinsic motivation0.87
Sub-dimensionsNegative
psychopathology
HappinessMotivation
Mixed symptoms0.75
Depressive symptoms0.87
Loss of interest0.77
BDI0.78
PANAS_NA0.73
Positive relations0.82
Autonomy0.420.54
Environmental mastery−0.440.74
Personal growth0.74
Purpose in life0.81
Self-acceptance−0.530.57
Challenge0.92
Intrinsic motivation0.87

Note: Factor loadings below 0.40 not shown.

We further explored whether such factors remained correlated with GPT and their mediating roles between GPT and aggression were robust (Table5). Results revealed that the negative psychopathology (factor 1) was positively correlated with GPT (r = 0.147, P = 0.003), physical (r = 0.144, P = 0.003), verbal (r = 0.147, P = 0.003), anger (r = 0.303, P < 0.001), hostility (r = 0.456, P < 0.001), indirect (r = 0.214, P < 0.001), aggreate score of BWAQ (r = 0.327, P < 0.001), reactive aggression (r = 0.411, P < 0.001) and proactive aggression (r = 0.226, P < 0.001) in the first dataset. Additionally, happiness (factor 2) was negatively correlated with GPT (r = −0.150, P = 0.002), physical (r = −0.206, P < 0.001), verbal (r = −0.187, P < 0.001), anger (r = −0.263, P < 0.001), hostility (r = −0.311, P < 0.001), aggregate score of BWAQ (r = −0.276, P < 0.001) and proactive aggression (r = −0.152, P = 0.002). However, we only observe a few small correlations of motivation (factor 3) with physical aggression (r = 0.109, P = 0.027), verbal aggression (r = 0.125, P = 0.011) and hostility (r = −0.120, P = 0.015) of BWAQ. Overall, these associations were replicated in the subgroup with high-quality imaging scans (n = 330) for negative psychopathology and happiness but not motivation (Table5).

Table5.

Correlations between three major factors and GPT/aggression scores

Dataset 1 (n = 411)Sub-dataset 1 (n = 330)
MeasuresF1F2F3F1F2F3
GPT0.147**−0.150**−0.0660.178**−0.182**−0.107
Physical0.144**−0.206***0.109*0.184**−0.232***0.096
Verbal0.147**−0.187***0.125*0.183**−0.224***0.108
Anger0.303***−0.263***−0.0710.305***−0.264***−0.049
Hostility0.456***−0.311***−0.120*0.434***−0.362***−0.067
Indirect0.214***−0.0840.0090.253***−0.0660.016
Total aggression0.327***−0.276***0.0120.348***−0.300***0.026
Reactive aggression0.411***−0.062−0.0280.441***−0.0950.038
Proactive aggression0.226***−0.152**−0.0140.297***−0.172**−0.012
Dataset 1 (n = 411)Sub-dataset 1 (n = 330)
MeasuresF1F2F3F1F2F3
GPT0.147**−0.150**−0.0660.178**−0.182**−0.107
Physical0.144**−0.206***0.109*0.184**−0.232***0.096
Verbal0.147**−0.187***0.125*0.183**−0.224***0.108
Anger0.303***−0.263***−0.0710.305***−0.264***−0.049
Hostility0.456***−0.311***−0.120*0.434***−0.362***−0.067
Indirect0.214***−0.0840.0090.253***−0.0660.016
Total aggression0.327***−0.276***0.0120.348***−0.300***0.026
Reactive aggression0.411***−0.062−0.0280.441***−0.0950.038
Proactive aggression0.226***−0.152**−0.0140.297***−0.172**−0.012

Abbreviations: F1, negative psychopathology; F2, happiness; F3, motivation. * P < 0.05;

**

P < 0.01;

***

P < 0.001.

Table5.

Correlations between three major factors and GPT/aggression scores

Dataset 1 (n = 411)Sub-dataset 1 (n = 330)
MeasuresF1F2F3F1F2F3
GPT0.147**−0.150**−0.0660.178**−0.182**−0.107
Physical0.144**−0.206***0.109*0.184**−0.232***0.096
Verbal0.147**−0.187***0.125*0.183**−0.224***0.108
Anger0.303***−0.263***−0.0710.305***−0.264***−0.049
Hostility0.456***−0.311***−0.120*0.434***−0.362***−0.067
Indirect0.214***−0.0840.0090.253***−0.0660.016
Total aggression0.327***−0.276***0.0120.348***−0.300***0.026
Reactive aggression0.411***−0.062−0.0280.441***−0.0950.038
Proactive aggression0.226***−0.152**−0.0140.297***−0.172**−0.012
Dataset 1 (n = 411)Sub-dataset 1 (n = 330)
MeasuresF1F2F3F1F2F3
GPT0.147**−0.150**−0.0660.178**−0.182**−0.107
Physical0.144**−0.206***0.109*0.184**−0.232***0.096
Verbal0.147**−0.187***0.125*0.183**−0.224***0.108
Anger0.303***−0.263***−0.0710.305***−0.264***−0.049
Hostility0.456***−0.311***−0.120*0.434***−0.362***−0.067
Indirect0.214***−0.0840.0090.253***−0.0660.016
Total aggression0.327***−0.276***0.0120.348***−0.300***0.026
Reactive aggression0.411***−0.062−0.0280.441***−0.0950.038
Proactive aggression0.226***−0.152**−0.0140.297***−0.172**−0.012

Abbreviations: F1, negative psychopathology; F2, happiness; F3, motivation. * P < 0.05;

**

P < 0.01;

***

P < 0.001.

Negative psychopathology and happiness factors correspondingly mediated the effect of GPT on aggression (Figure4). Negative psychopathology mediated the associations of GPT with physical aggression (all indirect effects > 0.017, 95% CI [0.002, 0.065]), verbal aggression (all indirect effects > 0.018, 95% CI [0.001, 0.063]), anger (all indirect effects > 0.042, 95% CI [0.016, 0.089]), hostility (all indirect effects > 0.061, 95% CI [0.026, 0.111]), indirect aggression (all indirect effects > 0.028, 95% CI [0.008, 0.086]), aggregate scores of BWAQ (all indirect effects > 0.043, 95% CI [0.017, 0.097]) and reactive aggression (all indirect effects > 0.058, 95% CI [0.023, 0.124]). Similarly, happiness mediated GPT effects on physical aggression (all indirect effects > 0.027, 95% CI [0.009, 0.073]), verbal aggression (all indirect effects > 0.024, 95% CI [0.006, 0.075]), anger (all indirect effects > 0.036, 95% CI [0.013, 0.084]), hostility (all indirect effects > 0.040, 95% CI [0.014, 0.099]) and aggregate scores of BWAQ (all indirect effects > 0.036, 95% CI [0.012, 0.086]). These mediation effects were robust in the subgroup with high-quality imaging scans (n = 330; all indirect effects > 0.01, 95% CI [0.006,0.124]).

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (7)

Fig.4.

Mediation models of two factors (i.e. negative psychopathology and happiness) on relationships between GPT and aggressions (including physical, verbal, anger, hostility, indirect, total scores of BWAQ, reactive aggression).

The morphological substrates of negative psychopathology and happiness

We further explored whether the mediation effects observed were also associated with brain morphological substrates underlying negative psycholopathology and happiness. First, VBM analysis revealed that negative psychopathology was positively associated with the GMVs in the left precentral gyrus (MNI = −7.25, −25, 74, Z = 3.65), right frontal operculum cortex (MNI = 37.6, 21, 6, Z = 2.98), right precentral gyrus (MNI = 28.9, −14.6, 69.9, Z = 3.42), right LOC (MNI = 17.8, −67.6, 55.8, Z = 3.73), left insular cortex (MNI = −34.4, 22, −6.33, Z = 3.02), left LOC (MNI = -9.62, −61.1,—67.8, Z = 3.55), right superior frontal gyrus (SFG; MNI = 8.08, 2.09, 72.6, Z = 3.13), left middle frontal gyrus (MFG; MNI = −30.7, 2.32, 64, Z = 3.44), left SFG (MNI = −21.4, 17.2, 47.5, Z = 2.51), right superior parietal lobule (SPL; MNI = 12.6, −46.7, 76.4, Z = 3.64), right ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC; MNI = 0.947, 16.1, 5.51, Z = 3.32) and left frontal orbital cortex (OFC; MNI = −19.3, 33.3, −22.3, Z = 2.73) (Table6) (Figure5). Moreover, negative psychopathology was negatively associated with GMVs in the frontal-temporal-occipital network, including the right middle temporal gyrus (MTG; MNI = 50.7, −26.4, −3.95, Z = 3.00), right precuneus cortex (MNI = 19.8, −66.2, 30.5, Z = 3.47), right occipital pole (OP; MNI = 13.1, −96.5, −10.3, Z = 3.27), left frontal pole (FPC; MNI = −34.5, 57.1, −8.66, Z = 3.53), left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; MNI = −46.1, 31, 18.2, Z = 3.33), left LOC (MNI = −30.2, −79.5, 2.54, Z = 3.19), right OFC (MNI = 13.7, 16.7, −16.4, Z = 2.65), left STG (MNI = −47.8, −0.775, −19.3, Z = 3.13), left MTG (MNI = −33.9, 23.4, 37.3, Z = 4.12) and right MFG (MNI = 39.7, 27.3, 22.7, Z = 2.81) (Table6) (Figure5).

Table6.

Statistical associations between GMVs and negative psychopathology

MNI coordinates
EffectBrain regionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Precentral Gyrus810−7.25−25743.65
R Frontal Operculum Cortex74237.62162.98
R Precentral Gyrus70128.9−14.669.93.42
R Lateral Occipital Cortex62517.8−67.655.83.73
L Insular Cortex451−34.422−6.333.02
L Lateral Occipital Cortex338−9.62−61.167.83.55
R Superior Frontal Gyrus2978.082.0972.63.13
L Middle Frontal Gyrus285−30.72.32643.44
L Superior Frontal Gyrus223−21.417.247.52.51
R Superior Parietal Lobule15612.6−46.776.43.64
R VMPFC1360.94716.15.513.32
L Frontal Orbital Cortex114−19.333.3−22.32.73
NegativeR Middle Temporal Gyrus108550.7−26.4−3.953.00
R Precuneus Cortex98319.8−66.230.53.47
R Occipital Pole68313.1−96.5−10.33.27
L Frontal Pole634−34.557.1−8.663.53
L Inferior Frontal Gyrus468−46.13118.23.33
L Lateral Occipital Cortex249−30.2−79.52.543.19
R Frontal Orbital Cortex20513.716.7−16.42.65
L Superior Temporal Gyrus205−47.8−0.775−19.33.13
L Middle Temporal Gyrus143−33.923.437.34.12
R Middle Frontal Gyrus12739.727.322.72.81
MNI coordinates
EffectBrain regionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Precentral Gyrus810−7.25−25743.65
R Frontal Operculum Cortex74237.62162.98
R Precentral Gyrus70128.9−14.669.93.42
R Lateral Occipital Cortex62517.8−67.655.83.73
L Insular Cortex451−34.422−6.333.02
L Lateral Occipital Cortex338−9.62−61.167.83.55
R Superior Frontal Gyrus2978.082.0972.63.13
L Middle Frontal Gyrus285−30.72.32643.44
L Superior Frontal Gyrus223−21.417.247.52.51
R Superior Parietal Lobule15612.6−46.776.43.64
R VMPFC1360.94716.15.513.32
L Frontal Orbital Cortex114−19.333.3−22.32.73
NegativeR Middle Temporal Gyrus108550.7−26.4−3.953.00
R Precuneus Cortex98319.8−66.230.53.47
R Occipital Pole68313.1−96.5−10.33.27
L Frontal Pole634−34.557.1−8.663.53
L Inferior Frontal Gyrus468−46.13118.23.33
L Lateral Occipital Cortex249−30.2−79.52.543.19
R Frontal Orbital Cortex20513.716.7−16.42.65
L Superior Temporal Gyrus205−47.8−0.775−19.33.13
L Middle Temporal Gyrus143−33.923.437.34.12
R Middle Frontal Gyrus12739.727.322.72.81

Notes: Positive and Negative represents positive and negative associations between GMVs and negative psychopathology.

Table6.

Statistical associations between GMVs and negative psychopathology

MNI coordinates
EffectBrain regionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Precentral Gyrus810−7.25−25743.65
R Frontal Operculum Cortex74237.62162.98
R Precentral Gyrus70128.9−14.669.93.42
R Lateral Occipital Cortex62517.8−67.655.83.73
L Insular Cortex451−34.422−6.333.02
L Lateral Occipital Cortex338−9.62−61.167.83.55
R Superior Frontal Gyrus2978.082.0972.63.13
L Middle Frontal Gyrus285−30.72.32643.44
L Superior Frontal Gyrus223−21.417.247.52.51
R Superior Parietal Lobule15612.6−46.776.43.64
R VMPFC1360.94716.15.513.32
L Frontal Orbital Cortex114−19.333.3−22.32.73
NegativeR Middle Temporal Gyrus108550.7−26.4−3.953.00
R Precuneus Cortex98319.8−66.230.53.47
R Occipital Pole68313.1−96.5−10.33.27
L Frontal Pole634−34.557.1−8.663.53
L Inferior Frontal Gyrus468−46.13118.23.33
L Lateral Occipital Cortex249−30.2−79.52.543.19
R Frontal Orbital Cortex20513.716.7−16.42.65
L Superior Temporal Gyrus205−47.8−0.775−19.33.13
L Middle Temporal Gyrus143−33.923.437.34.12
R Middle Frontal Gyrus12739.727.322.72.81
MNI coordinates
EffectBrain regionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Precentral Gyrus810−7.25−25743.65
R Frontal Operculum Cortex74237.62162.98
R Precentral Gyrus70128.9−14.669.93.42
R Lateral Occipital Cortex62517.8−67.655.83.73
L Insular Cortex451−34.422−6.333.02
L Lateral Occipital Cortex338−9.62−61.167.83.55
R Superior Frontal Gyrus2978.082.0972.63.13
L Middle Frontal Gyrus285−30.72.32643.44
L Superior Frontal Gyrus223−21.417.247.52.51
R Superior Parietal Lobule15612.6−46.776.43.64
R VMPFC1360.94716.15.513.32
L Frontal Orbital Cortex114−19.333.3−22.32.73
NegativeR Middle Temporal Gyrus108550.7−26.4−3.953.00
R Precuneus Cortex98319.8−66.230.53.47
R Occipital Pole68313.1−96.5−10.33.27
L Frontal Pole634−34.557.1−8.663.53
L Inferior Frontal Gyrus468−46.13118.23.33
L Lateral Occipital Cortex249−30.2−79.52.543.19
R Frontal Orbital Cortex20513.716.7−16.42.65
L Superior Temporal Gyrus205−47.8−0.775−19.33.13
L Middle Temporal Gyrus143−33.923.437.34.12
R Middle Frontal Gyrus12739.727.322.72.81

Notes: Positive and Negative represents positive and negative associations between GMVs and negative psychopathology.

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (8)

Fig.5.

Gray matter volume’s results related to negative psychophology and happiness.

In contrast, happiness was positively correlated with GMVs in the left postcentral gyrus (MNI = −10.8, −42.6, 58.3, Z= 2.83), but negatively correlated with GMVs in the prefronal-temporal-occipital-parietal network, including the left temporal pole (TP; MNI = −27.7, 11.7, −38.7, Z = 3.20), left MFG (MNI = -40.1, 24.4, 35.7, Z = 3.37), left lateral occipital pole (LOP; MNI = -35.2, −62.6, 34.8, Z = 4.03), left angular gyrus (MNI = −48.5, −51.1, 34.5, Z = 3.85), left LOC (MNI = −15, −66.4, 50.7, Z = 3.45), left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG; MNI = −48.1, −11, −43.3, Z = 3.03), right LOC (MNI = 42.9, −65.5, 39.8, Z = 2.93), right precentral gyrus (MNI = 56.5, 1.02, 15.7, Z = 2.98), left cingulate gyrus (MNI = −8.78, −49.1, 24.3, Z = 2.88) and right MFG (MNI = 29, 23, 33.1, Z = 2.70) (Table7) (Figure5).

Table7.

Statistical associations between GMVs and happiness

MNI Coordinates
EffectBrain RegionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Postcentral Gyrus134−10.8−42.658.32.83
NegativeL Temporal Pole1015−27.711.7−38.73.20
L Middle Frontal Gyrus668−40.124.435.73.37
L Lateral Occipital Pole330−35.2−62.634.84.03
L Angular Gyrus311−48.5−51.134.53.85
L Lateral Occipital Cortex300−15−66.450.73.45
L Inferior Temporal Gyrus261−48.1−11−43.33.03
R Lateral Occipital Cortex25542.9−65.539.82.93
R Precentral Gyrus17456.51.0215.72.98
L Cingulate Gyrus129−8.78−49.124.32.88
R Middle Frontal Gyrus35292333.12.70
MNI Coordinates
EffectBrain RegionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Postcentral Gyrus134−10.8−42.658.32.83
NegativeL Temporal Pole1015−27.711.7−38.73.20
L Middle Frontal Gyrus668−40.124.435.73.37
L Lateral Occipital Pole330−35.2−62.634.84.03
L Angular Gyrus311−48.5−51.134.53.85
L Lateral Occipital Cortex300−15−66.450.73.45
L Inferior Temporal Gyrus261−48.1−11−43.33.03
R Lateral Occipital Cortex25542.9−65.539.82.93
R Precentral Gyrus17456.51.0215.72.98
L Cingulate Gyrus129−8.78−49.124.32.88
R Middle Frontal Gyrus35292333.12.70

Notes: Positive and Negative represents positive and negative associations between GMVs and happiness.

Table7.

Statistical associations between GMVs and happiness

MNI Coordinates
EffectBrain RegionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Postcentral Gyrus134−10.8−42.658.32.83
NegativeL Temporal Pole1015−27.711.7−38.73.20
L Middle Frontal Gyrus668−40.124.435.73.37
L Lateral Occipital Pole330−35.2−62.634.84.03
L Angular Gyrus311−48.5−51.134.53.85
L Lateral Occipital Cortex300−15−66.450.73.45
L Inferior Temporal Gyrus261−48.1−11−43.33.03
R Lateral Occipital Cortex25542.9−65.539.82.93
R Precentral Gyrus17456.51.0215.72.98
L Cingulate Gyrus129−8.78−49.124.32.88
R Middle Frontal Gyrus35292333.12.70
MNI Coordinates
EffectBrain RegionCluster size (voxels)XYZZ
PositiveL Postcentral Gyrus134−10.8−42.658.32.83
NegativeL Temporal Pole1015−27.711.7−38.73.20
L Middle Frontal Gyrus668−40.124.435.73.37
L Lateral Occipital Pole330−35.2−62.634.84.03
L Angular Gyrus311−48.5−51.134.53.85
L Lateral Occipital Cortex300−15−66.450.73.45
L Inferior Temporal Gyrus261−48.1−11−43.33.03
R Lateral Occipital Cortex25542.9−65.539.82.93
R Precentral Gyrus17456.51.0215.72.98
L Cingulate Gyrus129−8.78−49.124.32.88
R Middle Frontal Gyrus35292333.12.70

Notes: Positive and Negative represents positive and negative associations between GMVs and happiness.

We further explored whether the GMVs in above-mentioned brain regions predicted individual variability in GPT and aggression. We found that the negative psychopathology-related FPC’s GMV was negatively associated with GPT (r = −0.132, P = 0.017), reactive aggression (r = −0.201, P < 0.001) and hostility of BWAQ (r = −0.152, P = 0.006). Similarly, the happiness-related GMVs in the left MFG (r = 0.134, P = 0.015), left angular gyrus (r = 0.136, P = 0.013) and right MFG (r = 0.114, P = 0.039) were positively associated with GPT. Furthermore, GMVs in the left MFG was positively correlated with physical aggression (r = 0.128, P = 0.02), while GMVs in the left angular gyrus was positively correlated with hostility aggression (r = 0.118, P = 0.032). The GMV in right MFG was found to be associated with physical (r = 0.174, P = 0.001), hostility (r = 0.145, P = 0.009) and aggregate scores of aggression (r = 0.146, P = 0.008).

We further examined whether the associations between GPT and aggressions were modulated by the GMVs in above-mentioned brain regions. Mediation models revealed that GMV in the FPC mediated the association between GPT and reactive aggression (indirect effect = 0.012, 95% CI = [0.002, 0.031]). Additionally, MFG’s volume mediated greedy individuals’ aggressions, including physical (indirect effect = 0.017, 95% CI [0.002, 0.044]), hostility (indirect effect = 0.012, 95% CI [0.000, 0.033]) and aggregate scores of aggression (indirect effect = 0.013, 95% CI [0.001, 0.035]) (Figure6).

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (9)

Fig.6.

Mediation models of brain regions on associations between GPT and aggression (FPC: Frontal Pole Cortex; MEG: Middle Frontal Gyrus).

Discussion

The present study comprehensively examined negative psychopathological symptomology as direct correlates and mediating mechanisms of dispositional greed (e.g. depression, mixed symptoms, loss of interest, negative affect) on its relation to maladaptive social behaviors (e.g. aggression). Further, EFA generated three factors, including negative psychopathology, happiness and motivation, two of which functioned as mediators. Brain imaging findings further revealed the neuroanatomical characteristics of negative psychopathology and happiness for the GMVs in the prefrontal-parietal-occipital system. Moreover, we found that behavioral mediation effects on the associations between greed and aggression depended on the brain morphological architecture, particularly the negative-feature-related FPC and happiness-trait-related MFG volumes. This situates the current work as one of the first to comprehensively delineate behavioral characteristics in relation to greed, and its potential mediating and neuroanatomical mechanisms, with particular focus on the prefrontal cortex.

Utilizing the MASQ as the initial exploratory step revealed notable associations between GPT and psychopathological symptomology, such as depression symptoms, mixed symptoms and loss of interest. To further validate these findings, we utilized other field-standard measures (i.e. BDI and BAI) to isolate depression and anxiety symptomology, showing evidence greedy individuals tended to exhibit more depression-related psychopathology. Further, using PANAS as a general proxy for affective states likewise observed a positive correlation between GPT and negative affect. Importantly, such core outcomes were also replicated in an independent dataset, implying the robust nature and co-occurrence of adverse affective experiences and negative psychopathology among greedy individuals. Our findings are largely consistent with past studies that have found similar negative behavioral and psychological consequences among greedy individuals across both directly and peripherally related domains, including increased envy (Winarick, 2010), low life satisfaction (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015), disrupted impression management (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015), adverse financial behavior (Seuntjens etal., 2016) and increased likelihood to accept bribes (Seuntjens etal., 2019). One potential reason for GPT’s association with negative emotion/affect may be due to pervasive upward social comparison caused by materialistic desire to have more (Balot, 2020). This long-term state of dissatisfaction and corresponding desire for an idealistic, yet likely unattainable lifestyle may further induce the loss of interest with one’s current environment, thereby manifesting as psychopathological symptomology (e.g. depression, anxiety and mixed symptoms).

The present study also investigated the relation between greed and different aspects of social aggression, including reactivity, physical aggression, anger, hostility and indirect aggression. Negative psychopathological symptomology mediated the effect of greed on aggression. Specifically, the two main factors extracted by EFA, i.e. negative psychopathology and happiness, validated these mediation effects. This suggests the importance of negative psychopathological symptomology on the explanation of greed’s psychological and social consequences. Indeed, several studies have likewise implied the influence of negative emotion and affect on aggression. Negative emotions triggered by exposure to averse situations were found to modulate aggressive behavior, suggesting an affective priming effect on aggression (Verona etal., 2002). In adolescents, the level of the daily negative emotions, such as anger, was associated with reactive aggression (Moore etal., 2019). On the other hand, positive emotions, such as happiness, have been associated with lower physical aggression (Ronen etal., 2013; Kılıçarslan and Liman, 2020). Taken together, greedy individuals may be more prone to aggress against others due to higher experience of negative emotions and low happiness stemming from pervasive dissatisfaction of not having enough.

EFA further revealed that there are three main latent constructs of negative psychopathology, happiness, and motivation, of which two revealed significant mediation effects of greed on aggression. Furthermore, VBM analysis showed the neuroanatomical substrates underlying negative psychopathology and happiness in the prefrontal-parietal-temporal-occipital system. In particular, the GMVs in the prefrontal-parietal network, including DLPFC, SPL, VMPFC, MFG, OFC and IFG, were associated with individual variability in negative psychopathology scores assessed by EFA, consistent with previous studies focusing on the neuroanatomical substrates of negative emotion and affect. Specifically, anxious/depressed symptoms were linked to the thickness in VMPFC and exhibited developmental characteristics (Ducharme etal., 2014; Newman etal., 2016). GMVs in cortical and subcortical cortices, including VMPFC, MPFC, ACC, IFG, insula and amygdala-hippocampus were found to be important for emotion and relevant regulation (Koven etal., 2010; Takeuchi etal., 2011; Killgore etal., 2012). Meta-analyses on neuroimaging studies have found that emotion-regulation (e.g. reappraisal) relies on several brain activations, including DLPFC, ventrolateral PFC, DMPFC, ACC and the parietal cortex (Kohn etal., 2014; Etkin etal., 2015), to decrease amygdala-related emotion brain activations. Moreover, Beck’s cognitive model of depression proposed that the functional and structural architectures on the aforementioned prefrontal cortices are the core brain regions that play critical roles on depression formation, including the modulation of the subcortical brain activations subserving into negative emotion processing, top-down cognitive control, and attention bias modification (Disner etal., 2011). Youths with bipolar disorder also exhibited specific GMV decreases in the lateral PFC, DLPFC, DMPFC and parahippocampal gyrus (Gold etal., 2016). Thus, the subcortical region, with particular emphasis on the amygdala, may hint at a potential top-down cognitive control processing important on negative psychopathology.

We also found that GMVs in negative-psychopathology-related FPC and happiness-related MFG could modulate the effects of greed on aggression, which further extends the neurobiological architecture of behavioral mediation effects and suggests that morphological organization might exert a critical role between greed and aggression. A recent study showed that the GMVs in the FPC were directly associated with individual variability in GPT (Wang etal., 2021a). Importantly, reward-related brain activations in the lateral OFC and prospective-thinking-related brain activations in the prefrontal network, including MFG, significantly predicted individual’s GPT scores (Wang etal., 2021a). Combining the functioning of these regions, we propose that negative psychopathology may modulate greed-related effects on aggression and this modulation effect may correspondingly depend on the prefrontal cortex morphological characteristics.

From the theoretical perspective, the current study not only directly examined the negative psychopathology core characteristics of greed possibly due to desiring more than normal need and the dissatisfaction of not have enough, but also further uncovered its adverse consequences on social behaviors (e.g. aggression) and potential neural substrates. Such findings further extend the understanding of the negative attributes of greed from traditional self-interest, materialism and maximization to negative emotion/affect and maladaptive social behaviors to neural mechanisms(Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015; Seuntjens etal., 2015b; Lambie and Haugen, 2019). It enables us to comprehensively and precisely understand the concept of greed from a theoretical view. At the practical level, it provides the possible strategies that focus on the emotion/affect-related manipulations and enhancements to further improve the life quality for greedy individuals. In addition, the current study is also valuable for shaping the economic behaviors in childhood and government management.

There are several limitations to consider in the present study. All findings of this study stem from a correlational design and do not allow for causal relations. Future studies may use experimental or longitudinal designs to examine the robustness of the current findings. Second, the behavioral data in this study were collected through self-report questionnaires which are subject to social desirability bias. Nonetheless, an independent sample provided consistent validation, increasing our confidence on the research conclusions. Third, the sample consisted of university students which may limit the extent to which these findings can be generalized to other populations. In addition, the morphological findings related to two main factors via EFA and mediation model cannot tell us specific functions and warrant further inquiry to understand the functional mechanisms of greed and its mediators on greed-aggression associations.

In conclusion, the present study systematically investigated the negative characteristics of dispositional greed and its relation to aggression, as well as its underlying neuroanatomical substrates. Our findings provided empirical evidence on greed-aggression association and observed that negative psychopathology, happiness, and GMVs in prefrontal cortex could mediate such associations. These findings improve our understanding of greed and the cognitive and neural mechanisms that may underly its role in behavioral aggression.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32000786, 31800920), Humanities and Social Science Fund Project of the Ministry of Education (20YJC190018), Natural Science Foundation of Tianjin City (20JCYBJC00920), and National College Students’ Innovation and Entrepreneurship Training Program (202110065023).

Conflict of interest

The authors declared that they had no conflict of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Research involving human participants

Ethnical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Tianjin Normal University and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Data and code availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Functional MRI Center at Tianjin Normal University (TJNU). Data and code are available from the corresponding authors with the permission of the TJNU.

References

Amabile

T.M.

,

Hill

K.G.

,

Hennessey

B.A.

,

Tighe

E.M.

(

1994

).

The Work Preference Inventory: assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

,

66

(

5

), 950.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Balot

R.K.

(

2020

).

Greed and Injustice in Classical Athens

.

Title is part of eBook package: De Gruyter Princeton eBook Package Backlist

2000

13

.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Beck

A.T.

,

Ward

C.H.

,

Mendelson

M.

,

Mock

J.

,

Erbaugh

J.

(

1961

).

An inventory for measuring depression

.

Archives of General Psychiatry

,

4

(

6

),

561

71

.doi:

.

Buss

A.H.

,

Warren

W.L.

(

2000

).

Aggression questionnaire:(AQ)

.

Torrence, CA

:

Western Psychological Services

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Clark

L.A.

,

Watson

D.

(

1991

).

Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications

.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

,

100

(

3

), 316.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Disner

S.G.

,

Beevers

C.G.

,

Haigh

E.A.

,

Beck

A.T.

(

2011

).

Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression

.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

,

12

(

8

),

467

77

.doi:

.

Donahue

J.J.

,

Goranson

A.C.

,

McClure

K.S.

,

Van Male

L.M.

(

2014

).

Emotion dysregulation, negative affect, and aggression: a moderated, multiple mediator analysis

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

70

,

23

8

.doi:

.

Ducharme

S.

,

Albaugh

M.D.

,

Hudziak

J.J.

, et al. (

2014

).

Anxious/depressed symptoms are linked to right ventromedial prefrontal cortical thickness maturation in healthy children and young adults

.

Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991)

,

24

(

11

),

2941

50

.doi:

.

Etkin

A.

,

Büchel

C.

,

Gross

J.J.

(

2015

).

The neural bases of emotion regulation

.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

,

16

(

11

),

693

700

.doi:

.

Fehr

E.

,

Gintis

H.

(

2007

).

Human motivation and social cooperation: experimental and analytical foundations

.

Annual Review of Sociology

,

33

,

43

64

.doi:

.

Garofalo

C.

,

Velotti

P.

(

2017

).

Negative emotionality and aggression in violent offenders: the moderating role of emotion dysregulation

.

Journal of Criminal Justice

,

51

,

9

16

.doi:

.

Gold

A.L.

,

Brotman

M.A.

,

Adleman

N.E.

, et al. (

2016

).

Comparing brain morphometry across multiple childhood psychiatric disorders

.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

,

55

(

12

),

1027

1037.e1023

.doi:

.

Hair

J.F.

, et al. (

2009

).

Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global Perspective. 7th ed.

Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Hardin

G.

(

1968

).

The tragedy of the commons

.

Science

,

162

(

3859

),

1243

8

.doi:

.

Hayes

A.F.

(

2017

).

Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach

.

Guilford Publications

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Jiang

X.

,

Hu

X.

,

Liu

Z.

,

Sun

X.

,

Xue

G.

(

2020

).

Greed as an adaptation to anomie: the mediating role of belief in a zero-sum game and the buffering effect of internal locus of control

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

152

, 109566.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Keyes

C.L.M.

(

1998

).

Social well-being

.

Social psychology quarterly

,

121

40

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Kılıçarslan

S.

,

Liman

B.

(

2020

).

Examining the relationship between happiness and aggression among adolescents

.

International Online Journal of Educational Sciences

,

12

,

244

62

.doi:

.

Killgore

W.

,

Weber

M.

,

Schwab

Z.

, et al. (

2012

).

Gray matter correlates of Trait and Ability models of emotional intelligence

.

Neuroreport

,

23

,

551

5

.doi:

.

Kohn

N.

,

Eickhoff

S.B.

,

Scheller

M.

,

Laird

A.R.

,

Fox

P.T.

,

Habel

U.

(

2014

).

Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation--an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis

.

Neuroimage

,

87

,

345

55

.doi:

.

Kovácsová

N.

,

Lajunen

T.

,

Rošková

E.

(

2016

).

Aggression on the road: relationships between dysfunctional impulsivity, forgiveness, negative emotions, and aggressive driving

.

Transportation Research. Part F, Traffic Psychology and Behaviour

,

42

,

286

98

.doi:

.

Koven

N.

,

Roth

R.

,

Garlinghouse

M.

,

Flashman

L.

,

Saykin

A.

(

2010

).

Regional gray matter correlates of perceived emotional intelligence

.

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

,

6

,

582

90

.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Krekels

G.

,

Pandelaere

M.

(

2015

).

Dispositional greed

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

74

,

225

30

.doi:

.

Lambie

G.W.

,

Haugen

J.S.

(

2019

).

Understanding greed as a unified construct

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

141

,

31

9

.doi:

.

Li

W.

,

Wang

H.

,

Xie

X.

,

Li

J.

(

2019

).

Neural mediation of greed personality trait on risk preference

.

ELIFE

,

8

, 1–15.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Liu

Z.

,

Sun

X.

,

Ding

X.

,

Hu

X.

,

Xu

Z.

,

Fu

Z.

(

2019

).

Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Dispositional Greed Scale and a portrait of greedy people

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

137

,

101

9

.doi:

.

Lyubomirsky

S.

,

Lepper

H.S.

(

1999

).

A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation

.

Social Indicators Research

,

46

(

2

),

137

55

.doi:

.

Moore

C.C.

,

Hubbard

J.A.

,

Bookhout

M.K.

,

Mlawer

F.

(

2019

).

Relations between reactive and proactive aggression and daily emotions in adolescents

.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

,

47

(

9

),

1495

507

.doi:

.

Mussel

P.

,

Reiter

A.M.F.

,

Osinsky

R.

,

Hewig

J.

(

2015

).

State- and trait-greed, its impact on risky decision-making and underlying neural mechanisms

.

Social Neuroscience

,

10

(

2

),

126

34

.doi:

.

Mussel

P.

,

Rodrigues

J.

,

Krumm

S.

,

Hewig

J.

(

2018

).

The convergent validity of five dispositional greed scales

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

131

,

249

53

.doi:

.

Mussel

P.

,

Hewig

J.

(

2016

).

The life and times of individuals scoring high and low on dispositional greed

.

Journal of Research in Personality

,

64

,

52

60

.doi:

.

Mussel

P.

,

Hewig

J.

(

2019

).

A neural perspective on when and why trait greed comes at the expense of others

.

Scientific Reports

,

9

.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Newman

E.

,

Thompson

W.K.

,

Bartsch

H.

, et al. (

2016

).

Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents

.

Brain Structure & Function

,

221

(

6

),

3013

25

.doi:

.

Pavot

W.

Diener

E.

(

2009

). Review of the Satisfaction With Life Scale. In:

Diener

,

E.

, (eds).

Assessing Well-Being

. Social Indicators Research Series,

39

,

Springer

:

Dordrecht

.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Podsakoff

P.M.

,

MacKenzie

S.B.

,

Lee

J.-Y.

,

Podsakoff

N.P.

(

2003

).

Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies

.

Journal of Applied Psychology

,

88

(

5

), 879.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Puhalla

A.A.

,

McCloskey

M.S.

(

2020

).

The relationship between physiological reactivity to provocation and emotion dysregulation with proactive and reactive aggression

.

Biological Psychology

,

155

, 107931.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Raine

A.

,

Dodge

K.

,

Loeber

R.

, et al. (

2006

).

The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys

.

Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression

,

32

(

2

),

159

71

.doi:

.

Ronen

T.

,

Abuelaish

I.

,

Rosenbaum

M.

,

Agbaria

Q.

,

Hamama

L.

(

2013

).

Predictors of aggression among Palestinians in Israel and Gaza: happiness, need to belong, and self-control

.

Children and Youth Services Review

,

35

(

1

),

47

55

.doi:

.

Ryff

C.D.

,

Keyes

C.L.

(

1995

).

The structure of psychological well-being revisited

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

,

69

(

4

),

719

27

.doi:

.

Seuntjens

T.G.

,

Zeelenberg

M.

,

Breugelmans

S.M.

,

van de Ven

N.

(

2015a

).

Defining greed

.

British Journal of Psychology

,

106

(

3

),

505

25

.doi:

.

Seuntjens

T.G.

,

Zeelenberg

M.

,

van de Ven

N.

,

Breugelmans

S.M.

(

2015b

).

Dispositional greed

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

,

108

(

6

),

917

33

.doi:

.

Seuntjens

T.G.

,

van de Ven

N.

,

Zeelenberg

M.

,

van der Schors

A.

(

2016

).

Greed and adolescent financial behavior

.

Journal of Economic Psychology

,

57

,

1

12

.doi:

.

Seuntjens

T.G.

,

Zeelenberg

M.

,

van de Ven

N.

,

Breugelmans

S.M.

(

2019

).

Greedy bastards: testing the relationship between wanting more and unethical behavior

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

138

,

147

56

.doi:

.

Takeuchi

H.

,

Taki

Y.

,

Sassa

Y.

, et al. (

2011

).

Regional gray matter density associated with emotional intelligence: evidence from voxel-based morphometry

.

Human Brain Mapping

,

32

(

9

),

1497

510

.doi:

.

Velotti

P.

,

Rogier

G.

,

Sarlo

A.

(

2019

).

Pathological narcissism and aggression: the mediating effect of difficulties in the regulation of negative emotions

.

Personality and Individual Differences

, 155.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Verona

E.

,

Patrick

C.J.

,

Lang

A.R.

(

2002

).

A direct assessment of the role of state and trait negative emotion in aggressive behavior

.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

,

111

(

2

), 249.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Vrabel

J.K.

,

Zeigler-Hill

V.

,

McCabe

G.A.

,

Baker

A.D.

(

2019

).

Pathological personality traits and immoral tendencies

.

Personality and Individual Differences

,

140

,

82

9

.doi:

.

Wang

P.

,

Feng

J.

,

Wang

Y.

, et al. (

2021a

).

Sex-specific static and dynamic functional networks of sub-divisions of striatum linking to the greed personality trait

.

Neuropsychologia

,

163

, 108066.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Wang

Q.

,

Lv

C.

,

He

Q.

,

Xue

G.

(

2020

).

Dissociable fronto-striatal functional networks predict choice impulsivity

.

Brain Structure & Function

,

225

(

8

),

2377

86

.doi:

.

Wang

Q.

,

Poh

J.S.

,

Wen

D.J.

, et al. (

2019a

).

Functional and structural networks of lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex as potential neural pathways for depression in childhood

.

Depression and Anxiety

,

36

(

4

),

365

74

.doi:

.

Wang

Q.

,

Wang

Y.

,

Wang

P.

, et al. (

2021b

).

Neural representations of the amount and the delay time of reward in intertemporal decision making

.

Human Brain Mapping

,

42

(

11

),

3450

69

.doi:

.

Wang

Q.

,

Wei

S.

,

Im

H.

, et al. (

2021c

).

Neuroanatomical and functional substrates of the greed personality trait

.

Brain Structure & Function

,

226

(

4

),

1269

80

.doi:

.

Wang

Q.

,

Zhang

H.

,

Wee

C.Y.

, et al. (

2019b

).

Maternal sensitivity predicts anterior hippocampal functional networks in early childhood

.

Brain Structure & Function

,

224

(

5

),

1885

95

.doi:

.

Watson

D.

,

Clark

L.A.

,

Tellegen

A.

(

1988

).

Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

,

54

(

6

), 1063.doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Williams

W.

(

2000

).

Greed versus compassion

.

Foundation for Economic Education-Working for a free and prosperous world

.

Winarick

K.

(

2010

).

Thoughts on greed and envy

.

The American Journal of Psychoanalysis

,

70

(

4

),

317

27

.doi:

.

Author notes

contributed equally.

© The Author(s) 2023. Published by Oxford University Press.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial reproduction and distribution of the work, in any medium, provided the original work is not altered or transformed in any way, and that the work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]

Advertisem*nt

Citations

Views

25,009

Altmetric

More metrics information

Metrics

Total Views 25,009

24,072 Pageviews

937 PDF Downloads

Since 7/1/2022

Month: Total Views:
July 2022 57
August 2022 7,799
September 2022 402
October 2022 547
November 2022 545
December 2022 325
January 2023 354
February 2023 462
March 2023 559
April 2023 796
May 2023 734
June 2023 569
July 2023 571
August 2023 646
September 2023 854
October 2023 1,004
November 2023 950
December 2023 1,363
January 2024 1,318
February 2024 1,025
March 2024 975
April 2024 675
May 2024 584
June 2024 453
July 2024 311
August 2024 732
September 2024 399

Citations

Powered by Dimensions

5 Web of Science

Altmetrics

×

Email alerts

Article activity alert

Advance article alerts

New issue alert

Receive exclusive offers and updates from Oxford Academic

Citing articles via

Advertisem*nt

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates (2024)

FAQs

Greed personality trait links to negative psychopathology and underlying neural substrates? ›

Correlational analyses revealed that greedy individuals show more negative symptoms (e.g. depression, loss of interest, negative affect), lower psychological well-being and more aggression.

What is the greed personality trait? ›

Greed personality trait (GPT), characterized by the desire to acquire more and the dissatisfaction of never having enough, has been hypothesized to link with negative emotion/affect characteristics and aggressive behaviors.

What is the psychological disorder greed? ›

As a psychiatric diagnosis, it could be called the Great Gatsby Syndrome or, better yet, Wealth Accumulation Disorder. Both narcissism and greed have their roots in profound self-doubt. Narcissism is self-aggrandizement of the emotional kind, while greed is self-aggrandizement of the materialistic kind.

How does greed affect the brain? ›

It uncovers that high levels of GPT correlate with increased depression, anger, and aggression. This correlation extends beyond behavior to the brain's structure, as neuroimaging data indicates significant impacts on specific brain regions in those with higher greed traits.

Is greed a negative emotion? ›

Greed and envy are like identical twins in many ways. Both are complex human emotions with severe negative psychological effects. They are also closely connected to the desire to acquire, whether it's wealth, possessions, status or recognition.

What are the negative qualities of greed? ›

Correlational analyses revealed that greedy individuals show more negative symptoms (e.g. depression, loss of interest, negative affect), lower psychological well-being and more aggression.

What is the root cause of greed? ›

Greed can stem from emotional trauma and unmet needs. In a bid to replace the emptiness of emotional distress, a person can attempt to use objects or possessions.

Is greed a form of narcissism? ›

Narcissism isn't just greed for the sake of pleasure; it's also greed for the sake of comfort. Some vulnerable narcissists consider themselves to be special because of years of suffering, or due to some horrific experience, therefore, they come to believe, the world owes all of its attention to them.

What emotion is associated with greed? ›

Considerable empirical evidence has also demonstrated that greedy individuals subjectively experience a series of negative emotions, including unhappiness (Seuntjens et al., 2015a), envy (Krekels and Pandelaere, 2015), negative affect after losing money (Mussel and Hewig, 2016), life dissatisfaction (Pavot and Diener, ...

What are the symptoms of a greedy person? ›

  • Overly self-centered behaviour becomes the first give-away of greedy people. ...
  • Envy and greed are like twins. ...
  • Greedy people lack empathy. ...
  • They are never satisfied. ...
  • Greedy people are experts in manipulation.
Apr 8, 2016

How does greed change a person? ›

Far too often, greed comes with stress, exhaustion, anxiety, depression and despair. In addition, it can lead to maladaptive behaviour patterns such as gambling, hoarding, trickery and even theft.

Where does greed lead a person? ›

Unrestrained greed in an individual can lead to callousness, arrogance, and even megalomania. A person dominated by greed will often ignore the harm their actions can cause others.

What is the psychology of a greedy person? ›

At the neurological level, greed is controlled by the reward centre of the brain. Greedy people feel good when they choose the stuff they want, and this happens at the unconscious, emotional level of the brain, meaning there's little conscious awareness about how greedy actions might affect others, or be unfair.

How to deal with a greedy person? ›

Dealing with greedy people involves setting clear boundaries, communicating assertively, and prioritizing your own well-being. Recognize their behavior, avoid enabling them, and if necessary, distance yourself from toxic relationships.

Is greed worse than envy? ›

While it is subjective to say whether one is inherently worse than the other, envy is more corrosive and harmful than greed for several reasons. What is envy? Envy arises when one person desires what someone else has and feels resentful about not possessing it themselves.

Is greed a coping mechanism? ›

Seltzer, Ph. D., argued that greed, like addiction, is often a coping mechanism for unresolved mental health issues. By obtaining incredible wealth or success, people with deep insecurities strive to feel like they are finally good enough, or at least better than their peers.

What personality type is greedy? ›

Greedy is an INFP and Enneagram Type 8w7. Debate the personality types of your favorite fictional characters and celebrities. "Anything that's worth doing... is worth doing for myself!"

What is the character of a greedy person? ›

Greedy people are not good at maintaining boundaries. They will compromise moral values and ethics to achieve their goals. They look for loopholes or clever ways to outsmart the rules and regulations that have been put into place to moderate this kind of behaviour.

What is the human trait greed? ›

Greed personality trait (GPT) is often characterized by the experience of desiring more and the dissatisfaction of not having enough.

What describes a greedy person? ›

If you describe someone as greedy, you mean that they want to have more of something such as food or money than is necessary or fair. He attacked greedy bosses for awarding themselves big raises. Synonyms: avaricious, grasping, selfish, insatiable More Synonyms of greedy. greedily adverb [ADV with v]

Top Articles
Why Not Trade Forex on Monday and Friday? - Tekedia
Is it Too Late to Start a Career in Real Estate?
Automated refuse, recycling for most residences; schedule announced | Lehigh Valley Press
Chs.mywork
Meer klaarheid bij toewijzing rechter
Call Follower Osrs
Call Follower Osrs
Caresha Please Discount Code
What Happened To Maxwell Laughlin
How do you like playing as an antagonist? - Goonstation Forums
Evil Dead Rise Showtimes Near Regal Columbiana Grande
2016 Ford Fusion Belt Diagram
Becu Turbotax Discount Code
Pretend Newlyweds Nikubou Maranoshin
Keci News
Air Quality Index Endicott Ny
Gotcha Rva 2022
Greyson Alexander Thorn
Walgreens Bunce Rd
Pawn Shop Moline Il
Copper Pint Chaska
Section 408 Allegiant Stadium
Angel del Villar Net Worth | Wife
Hoofdletters voor God in de NBV21 - Bijbelblog
Citibank Branch Locations In Orlando Florida
R3Vlimited Forum
Wega Kit Filtros Fiat Cronos Argo 1.8 E-torq + Aceite 5w30 5l
2024 Coachella Predictions
EST to IST Converter - Time Zone Tool
Green Bay Crime Reports Police Fire And Rescue
Robot or human?
Tas Restaurant Fall River Ma
Hair Love Salon Bradley Beach
The 38 Best Restaurants in Montreal
Spectrum Outage in Genoa City, Wisconsin
Cygenoth
A Comprehensive 360 Training Review (2021) — How Good Is It?
Author's Purpose And Viewpoint In The Dark Game Part 3
Man Stuff Idaho
Executive Lounge - Alle Informationen zu der Lounge | reisetopia Basics
814-747-6702
VDJdb in 2019: database extension, new analysis infrastructure and a T-cell receptor motif compendium
White County
Goats For Sale On Craigslist
Gabrielle Abbate Obituary
R/Gnv
Whitney Wisconsin 2022
Oefenpakket & Hoorcolleges Diagnostiek | WorldSupporter
Bradshaw And Range Obituaries
Skybird_06
Bob Wright Yukon Accident
Swissport Timecard
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Manual Maggio

Last Updated:

Views: 5904

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (49 voted)

Reviews: 80% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Manual Maggio

Birthday: 1998-01-20

Address: 359 Kelvin Stream, Lake Eldonview, MT 33517-1242

Phone: +577037762465

Job: Product Hospitality Supervisor

Hobby: Gardening, Web surfing, Video gaming, Amateur radio, Flag Football, Reading, Table tennis

Introduction: My name is Manual Maggio, I am a thankful, tender, adventurous, delightful, fantastic, proud, graceful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.